Studies on which the examples are based


Example 1:
Roberts, S. (1981).
Isolation of an internal clock.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 7,
242 - 268.

Example 2:
Osman, A., Bashore, T. R., Coles, M. G. H., Donchin, E., & Meyer, D. E. (1992).
On the transmission of partial information: inferences from movement-related brain potentials.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 18,
217-232.

Example 3:
McCarthy, D., & Davison, M. (1984).
Isobias and alloiobias functions in animal psychophysics.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 10,
390-409.

Example 4:
Graham, N. (1972).
Spatial frequency channels in the human visual system: effects of luminance and pattern drift rate.
Vision Research 12,
53-68.

Example 5:
Mouret, I., & Hasbroucq, T. (2000).
The chronometry of single neuron activity: testing discrete and continuous models of information processing.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
(in press).

Example 7:
Sachs, M. B., Nachmias, J., & Robson, J. G. (1971).
Spatial-frequency channels in human vision.
Journal of the Optical Society of America, 61,
1176-1186.

Example 8:
Kounios, J. (1999).
Neurocognitive modules revealed by event-related brain potentials.
Manuscript submitted for publication.

Example 9:
Roberts, S. (1987).
Evidence for distinct serial processes in animals: the multiplicative-factors method.
Animal Learning and Behavior 15,
135-173.

Example 10:
Sanders, A. F., Wijnen, J. L. C., & Van Arkel, A. E. (1982).
An additive factor analysis of the effects of sleep loss on reaction processes.
Acta Psychologica 51,
41-59.

Example in Appendix A6:
Smulders, F. T. Y., Kok, A., Kenemans, J. L., & Bashore, T. R. (1995).
The temporal selectivity of additive factor effects on the reaction process revealed in ERP component latencies.
Acta Psychologica, 90,
97-109.