Example 2:
Osman, A., Bashore, T. R., Coles, M. G. H., Donchin, E., & Meyer, D. E.
(1992).
On the transmission of partial information: inferences from
movement-related brain potentials.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 18,
217-232.
Example 3:
McCarthy, D., & Davison, M.
(1984).
Isobias and alloiobias functions in animal psychophysics.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 10,
390-409.
Example 4:
Graham, N.
(1972).
Spatial frequency channels in the human visual
system: effects of luminance and pattern drift rate.
Vision Research 12,
53-68.
Example 5:
Mouret, I., & Hasbroucq, T.
(2000).
The chronometry of single neuron activity: testing discrete
and continuous models of information processing.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
(in press).
Example 7:
Sachs, M. B., Nachmias, J., & Robson, J. G. (1971).
Spatial-frequency channels in human vision.
Journal of the Optical Society of America, 61,
1176-1186.
Example 8:
Kounios, J.
(1999).
Neurocognitive modules revealed by event-related brain potentials.
Manuscript submitted for publication.
Example 9:
Roberts, S.
(1987).
Evidence for distinct serial processes in animals:
the multiplicative-factors method.
Animal Learning and Behavior 15,
135-173.
Example 10:
Sanders, A. F., Wijnen, J. L. C., & Van Arkel, A. E. (1982).
An additive factor analysis of the effects of sleep loss on reaction
processes.
Acta Psychologica 51,
41-59.
Example in Appendix A6:
Smulders, F. T. Y., Kok, A., Kenemans, J. L., & Bashore, T. R.
(1995).
The temporal selectivity of additive factor effects on the reaction
process revealed in ERP component latencies.
Acta Psychologica, 90,
97-109.