homepage    mcep   e-portfolio                  <-- PREVIOUS REFLECTION                NEXT REFLECTION -->
  Home            MCEP home     E-portfolio


Reflection 6:  Understanding of Science Education Theory and Literature

How the rubric item was addressed in this reflection:
This reflection shows how I have grown to have a stronger understanding of important education literature and theory, to demonstrate that I have studied and been significantly affected by some aspect of science education literature and theory as a result of a program course.

WHAT is the evidence? WHY did I choose the evidence? HOW does the evidence show growth?

Before entering the MCE program, I had no previous experience with science education literature.  Through my participation in the two education courses Edu536 and Edu636, I have progressively grown to have a stronger understanding of the constructivist nature of learning, specifically the importance of addressing learner preconceptions. 

I present 1 piece of baseline evidence from Edu536 and 1 piece of later evidence from Edu636.  In comparing my baseline and later evidence, I use a conceptual framework that shows:

  • STRONGER UNDERSTANDING OF CONSTRUCTIVISM & THE ROLE OF PRECONCEPTIONS:  My increase in understanding of constructivism can best be seen by comparing the progression in my treatment of the constructivist framework in my research proposal  for Edu536 (baseline evidence) and in selected articles from my annotated bibliography for Edu636 (later evidence). Specifically, I grew to better understand the role learners' pre-existing knowledge plays in the process of concept invention.  As a result of a better understanding of constructivism, I also modified my instruction (See Reflection 8) and my assessment practices (See Reflection 9).

  • CONSTRUCTIVISM is the theory of learning that states that learners continuously reconstruct knowledge networks by comparing and contrasting previously learned information with newly encountered information.  In other words, sense is made of new knowledge by interacting with and processing this information in light of prior knowledge.  When newly learned information is integrated into the framework with previously learned information, meaningful learning has occurred.  To correct naive conceptions, previously held ideas must be explicitly evoked and tested in the context of the new information.
`

Baseline Evidence:
The constructivist framework
in my research proposal for Edu536
MAY 2008

framework

This baseline evidence represents what I understood of constructivist learning theory from my first exposure to it in Edu536.  I reference 3 articles (Johnstone, 1993; Spencer, 1999; and More, 2007), but I did not yet fully grasp the role of preconceptions in determining how students learn.  I understood that students constructed their own understanding, and I knew that new information had to fit into "old" information, but I did not fully appreciate the necessity of evoking and addressing preconceptions in the concept invention process.

References:

Johnstone, A. H. (1983). Chemical Education Research: Facts, Findings, and Consequences. Journal of Chemical Education, 60(11), 968-971.

Spencer, J. N. (1999). New Directions in Teaching Chemistry: A Philosophical and Pedagogical Basis.  Journal of Chemical Education, 76(4), 566-569.

More, M.B. (2007). Terra Firma: “Physics First” for Teaching Chemistry to Pre-Service Elementary School Teachers. Journal of Chemical Education, 84(4), 622-625.



Later Evidence:
Selected articles from
my annotated bibliography for Edu636
MAR 2009

Roth, W-M. (2006). Ch 1: Aporias of perception in science.  Learning Science: A Singular Plural Perspective.  Rotterdam: Sense Publications, 27-66.
    In this chapter of his book, Roth examines the usefulness of science demonstrations in the classroom.  Roth examines the differing perspectives that instructors and students have regarding the phenomena these demonstrations seek to illuminate.  In most cases, demonstrations alone were useless in creating an understanding of phenomena when unpaired with proper context and guided inquiry.  Instead, demonstrations could even foster incorrect notions or merely detract from the conceptual understanding they were seeking to reinforce. Since the perception of events and the causality assigned to them is dependent upon current understandings and exposure, Roth highlights the need to explore the phenomenon, seek explanations from students, and to re-iterate the process. This article was disturbing and eye-opening to me because I realized that while demonstrations (or any colorful or engaging pratice) are exciting and fun, they are useless when devoid of pedagogical practices that foster inquiry and the correction of wrongful understandings.  It reinforced the absolute necessity of building exploration, inquiry, assessment, and repetition into activities.

Johnstone, A.H. (1997). Chemistry teaching--Science or alchemy? Journal of Chemical Education, 74(3), 262-268.
    Alexander Johnstone (recipient of the 2009 ACS Award for Achievement in Research for the Teaching and Learning of Chemistry) succinctly re-presents an information processing model taken from education, psychology, and artificial intelligence theories that fits within the constructivist framework of learning.  Information is learned only after it has been strained through a perceptive filter into short-term memory where it is interpreted and processed by interaction with prior information retrieved from long-term memory.  Only after the new information is placed in context with previous information is it placed in long-term memory (i.e. learned) for future retrieval and application.  Because the amount of short-term memory available is a constant, to increase the efficiency of recall and storage, students must increase their ability to organize or "chunk" many separate pieces of information into a more manageable number of coherent, related groups of information.  A student's preconceptions and lack of experience combined with an abundance of new, seemingly unrelated information can overload his ability to achieve meaningful, long-term learning.  In light of this model, Johnston suggests that instructors abide by ten commandments of instructionThis article has been particularly important in giving me a foundational model for understanding how students learn and, in particular, how they process new information in the context of previously learned information.

Nakhleh, M.B.(1992).  Why some students don't learn chemistry: Chemical misconceptions. Journal of Chemical Education, 69(3), 191-196.
   
Nakhleh identifies student misconceptions across age groups (high school to undergraduate level) with the kinetic and particulate nature of matter (KPNR) and its implications to several other concepts in chemistry: phase changes, equilibrium, reactions, equations.  Examples and illustrations of student misconceptions are given.  This article is of particular interest to me because it highlights the difficulty with getting students to understand the submicroscopic nature of chemistry.  In light of the previous articles that showed how concrete real-life experience is often the easiest way to create coherent and meaningful learning, it is no wonder that students have difficulty understanding an aspect of chemistry that cannot be seen, but only imagined or experienced on the macroscopic level!  At the same time, because KPNR is such a cornerstone of chemistry, whatever means necessary must be taken to increase student understanding, whether that involves the use of manipulatives or repetitive connection with and interpretation of macroscopic phenomena in terms of KPNR.

In contrast, the later evidence shows that I have thought deeply about the constructivist nature of learning and the obstacles that students face in constructing accurate conceptual understandings.  The three articles are of particular interest and importance to me because together, they helped catalyze an awareness of the need to specifically assess and address student misconceptions.  Through class discussions and the creation of the annotated bibliography in Edu636, I began to think about how all the articles I had been reading fit together. 

After having read Roth's 2006 chapter on the "aporias of perception" after a professional development on demonstrations, I realized that even excellent (in my eyes), entertaining, and well-thought out instructional practices could fail if they did not adequately address students' pre-existing ideas.  The "aporias of perception" that Roth mentions occur when students learn or reinforce ideas that are quite different from the ones the teacher intended to teach and thought were successfully taught.  I was shocked by the idea that if I did not not adequately acknowledge the misconceptions or naive conceptual understandings that students bring with them into the classroom, my lesson would be completely would be completely useless in promoting learning objectives. 

The epiphany brought about by reading Roth's chapter was reinforced by articles I had already read by Nakhleh (1992) and Johnstone (1997).  I decided to re-read and re-evaluate the articles written by Johnstone (1997) and Nakhleh (1992).  I realized that Johnstone had been stressing the importance of addressing previously learned information, but I had not fully understood what he had meant at that time because I had thought that if a lesson were planned well it would naturally address student preconceptions, whereas I now know that pre-assessment and formative assessments have to be deliberately built into one's lesson plan.



Return to top
July 1, 2009